Photo Credit: CrimeAfterCrime.com
|
January 23, 2012
“We see what we want to see,” my
grandmother used to say. This insight visited me recently after I ran
across the mall chasing a woman I thought was my cousin. It wasn’t, as
it turned out, but I didn’t realize that until after I had puffed up
behind her, bopped her amiably on the shoulder and cried out, “Boo!”
How
was it possible, I thought in retrospective embarrassment, to so
wrongly misidentify someone I know so well? Empirically my experience
was all too common. I’d been thinking about my cousin a few moments
before and saw the woman through the lens of those thoughts. We often
project our life’s associations onto the faces of strangers.
Constantly—if mostly unconsciously—we familiarize them with learned
stereotypes. If we are wise, we learn to take caution with our
assumptions. We recognize this innate fallibility, and most of the time
it doesn’t matter very much.
Oddly
enough, however, we reverse that supposition in the one context where
fallibility matters most: in criminal cases, eyewitness testimony is
viewed as the ne plus ultra for the prosecution, despite a
century’s worth of psychological and sociological studies revealing
that, from Sacco and Vanzetti to Troy Davis, witnesses misperceive a
startling percentage of the time.
“Human beings are not very good at
identifying people they saw only once for a relatively short period of
time,” writes Cornell law professor Michael Dorf. “The studies reveal
error rates of as high as fifty percent—a frightening statistic given
that many convictions may be based largely or solely on such testimony.
These studies show further that the ability to identify a stranger is
diminished by stress (and what crime situation is not intensely
stressful?), that cross-racial identifications are especially
unreliable, and that contrary to what one might think, those witnesses
who claim to be ‘certain’ of their identifications are no better at it
than everyone else, just more confident.”
No comments:
Post a Comment